An Introduction To Relevance-Theoretic Pragmatics
An Introduction To Relevance-Theoretic Pragmatics
An Introduction To Relevance-Theoretic Pragmatics
THEORETIC PRAGMATICS
1.1 Introduction
one of signs used in the communication, the user of the signs which human is one
of the users, and the context of situation in which the communication occurs. For
addition, the aspects of sign used in the communication are discussed in the
discipline of semiotics.
semantics, and pragmatics. “First, syntax covers discussions about the relatioship
between signs and within the sign system. Second, semantics covers discussions
about the relationship between signs and the objects they refer to. Third,
pragmatics covers discussions about the relationship between signs and the
Pragmatics deals with the relationship between signs and their users.
user of signs, encodes and infers from the language, as a sign. Furthermore, the
aspects are seen as closely influenced by the context of situation and the
background knowledge of the human. Therefore, pragmatics also deals with some
Relevance theory, which was firstly proposed by Deirdre Wilson & Dan
central principle. It states that human’s cognitive system tends to take the most
Moreover, the notion of relevance is deeply discussed in its close relation with
the human’s cognitive system in correlation with the context of situation and
human’s cognition.
automatically create expectations which guide the hearer towards the speaker’s
meaning.” It is quite obvious that relevance theory was constructed under the
has been developed in such a way, so the relevance theory is said to explain a
relevance theory. Hopefully, this paper wil be helpful for a beginner who are
starting to enter the very wide scope of ideas of Sperber and Wilson in their
relevance theory.
CHAPTER II
DISCUSSION
explained through linguistic analysis only. For instance, a mother who orders her
child to take a broom in a corner of their house by saying “please!” while pointing
her fingers to the broom. The utterance above does not make sense in term of
explained by both phrase structure rules and transformational rules. It does not
have noun phrase that can be generated into deep structure, so there is nothing to
utterance based on Chomsky’s account. However, the child can understand what
his mother want and soon he takes the broom for his mother. How can it be?
(2002: 1) stated “Relevance theory may be seen as an attempt to work out in detail
structure and meaning, but also containing the intention of its speaker or writer
example, the mother’s utterance contains more than what is uttered, it contains the
mother’s intention that can be catched by the child. This phenomena is explained
input is a nature of the cognition itself which is guided by the cognitive principle
statement that the ability to recognize whether an input is relevant or not is the
the human cognition. The process can be in the form of memorizing what one has
already known about certain things, exploring the knowledge about inputs he/she
gets, answering questions he/she has in mind, etc. All of those processes
cognitive effects. The positive cognitive effects can be resulted from the
confirmation of the input, the mental representatives of the world, and the context
of situation. For instance, Hasbul is offering a glass of coffee to his friend Syaiful
by saying “ful, coffee!” while showing up his glass of coffee to syaiful. in this
case, Syaiful may understand the intention of hasbul by processing the inputs (the
utterance and the attidtude of hasbul) with the knowledge he has already had and
the context of situation. Syaiful might have already known that someone who
calls other’s name and shows a glass of coffee means he/she is offering it to
him/her. Also, his knowledge is confirmed with the input and the context. Thus,
this confirmation yields a positive cognitive effect for him, then syaiful will
more relevant than others. Sperber and Wilson (2002: 4) stated “In relevance-
theoretic terms, other things being equal, the greater the positive cognitive effects
achieved by processing an input, the greater its relevance will be.” Thus,
cognitive effects mean the greater confirmation resulted from the process of
situation. In the example above, Syaiful will have a greater positive effects if
Hasbul offers the coffee while saying “Ful, let’s enjoy this coffee together!” since
Syaiful will have stronger confirmation than the first example. Thus, in the
cognitive process. Sperber and Wilson stated (2002: 4) “other things being equal,
the greater the PROCESSING EFFORT required, the less relevant the input will
be.” Intuitively, the harder an input is to perceive, infer, or conclude, the less the
1. Other things being equal, the greater the positive cognitive effects achieved by
processing an input, the greater the relevance of the input to the individual at
that time.
2. Other things being equal, the greater the processing effort expended, the lower
“Mary, who dislikes most meat and is allergic to chicken, rings her dinner
party host to find out what is on the menu. He could truly tell her any of
three things:
would be relevant to Mary, but (3) would be more relevant than either (2)
or (4). It would be more relevant than (2) for reasons of cognitive effect:
(3) entails (2), and therefore yields all the conclusions derivable from (2),
and more besides. It would be more relevant than (4) for reasons of
processing effort: although (3) and (4) are logically equivalent, and
therefore yield exactly the same cognitive effects, these effects are
easier to derive from (3) than from (4), which requires an additional effort
of parsing and inference (in order to work out that the second disjunct is
false and the first is therefore true). Thus, (3) would be the most relevant
utterance to Mary, for reasons of both effort and effect. More generally,
when similar amounts of effort are required, the effect factor is decisive in
input it gets. When one is given a choice to pick some relevant inputs, he/she will
choose the most relevant among others. Therefore, those assumptions about
maximisation of relevance.”
stimulus it gets makes it possible for someone to predict what someone else thinks
intention to others he/she will choose the most relevant way in order to make
his/her stimulus as worth-processing for the hearer. Then, he/she will make an
overt or intentional way to show that the inputs he/she wants to communicate is
OSTENSIVE-INFERENTIAL COMMUNICATION.
explained as follows: the speaker will use the overt way in communicating his/her
intention, that is actively helping the hearer to recognise his/her INFORMATIVE
INTENTION.
engaging in such an act unless the audience pays attention to it. But equally, from
to be worth the audience’s processing effort; (b) it is the most relevant one
B chooses the expression as the most relevant ostensive stimulus for A to inform
RELEVANCE. Thus, A will consider that B’s utterance is the most relevant input,
EFFECT from the input of B’s utterance. Then in turn, A will process it in
relationship with his background knowledge and the context of situation. The
B’s utterance)
In the end, A can understand the implicit meaning of B’s utterance. Or, he
can identify the intention of B, that B is actually refusing his request. The above
in the example above. Conventionally, B should simply answer the A’s request by
“yes or no” In fact, B’s utterance is not conventionally expected. Furthermore, the
discussion, the theory can explain such phenomenon in litarature as like in poetry,
prose, etc. Of course, it is done with more complex analysis then that is presented
in this paper.
CHAPTER III
CONCLUSION
3.1 Conclusion
contain the intention of the speaker. Thus, the communication is not only
This claim seems identical with Grice’s central claim about the
theory and Grice’s maxims. Specifically, the difference appears on the notion of
of any ostensively conveyed input to reach its optimal relevance by its own way.
It explains why some phenomenon of communication can occur well despite its
complex features.
REFERENCES
Company.
Sperber, Dan. & Wilson, Deirdre. 1993. Linguistic Form and Relevance. Lingua,
90(1): 1-25.
Sperber, Dan. & Wilson, Deirdre. 2002. Relevance Theory. In L. Horn & G. Ward
Publishing Ltd.