Drooling, saliva
production, and
swallowing in
cerebral palsy
Jill E Senner* PhD CCC-SLP;
Jerilyn Logemann PhD CCC-SLP;
Steven Zecker PhD CCC-SLP, Northwestern University,
Evanston, Northwestern Campus;
Deborah Gaebler-Spira MD, Rehabilitation Institute of
Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
*Correspondence to first author at Communication
Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, 2240
Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA.
E-mail:
[email protected]
Fourteen participants (six females, eight males) ranging in
age from 7 years 11 months to 18 years 2 months (mean 11y
7mo) with a confirmed diagnosis of spastic cerebral palsy
(CP) were included in the study. Participants included those
who drooled (CP+, n=14); age- and sex-matched children
with spastic CP who were dry to mild and never to infrequent
droolers (CP–, n=14) as well as typically developing peers
(CTRL, n=14) served as controls. Frequency of swallowing
was measured by using simultaneous cervical ausculation and
videotaping of the head and neck. Saliva production was
measured with the Saxon test, a simple gauze-chewing
procedure. In addition, Pediatric Evaluation of Disability
Inventory (PEDI), Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-3 (TONI-3),
dysarthria severity scale, and Gross Motor Function
Classification System (GMFCS) scores were obtained for each
participant. Both groups of participants with CP tended to
swallow less frequently than typically developing participants
and tended to produce less saliva than typically developing
controls; however, these differences were not statistically
significant. No correlation was found between amount of
saliva produced and amount drooled (r=0.245). An analysis
of variance (ANOVA) conducted on the PEDI functional
skills mean scores indicated significant differences between
the three groups (F(2,39)=23.522, p<0.0001). Likewise, an
ANOVA conducted on the TONI-3 scores revealed statistically
significant differences between the three groups (F(2,39)=31.761,
p<0.0001). A Spearman’s rho correlation indicated that GMFCS
scores were not significantly correlated with drooling severity
(Spearman’s rho correlation=0.3951, p=0.037). Drooling
severity was found to be positively correlated with dysarthria
severity (Spearman’s rho correlation=0.82, p<0.0001). These
findings suggest that drooling in patients with CP is related to
swallowing difficulties rather than hypersalivation.
See list of abbreviations at end of paper.
Drooling, ‘spilling of saliva from the mouth onto the lips, chin,
neck, and clothing’ (Brodsky 1993, p389), normally occurs in
infants and young children, particularly when a child is learning a new motor skill or cutting a new tooth. By the time typically developing children are 24 months old, they should
have the ability to perform most activities without drooling
(Morris and Klein 1987). It has been estimated that drooling
abnormally persists in 10 to 38% of individuals with cerebral
palsy (CP; Johnson and Scott 1993). Consequences of drooling include irritated facial skin, unpleasant odor, increased
oral and perioral infections, hygiene problems, and dehydration (Cotton and Richardson 1981, Harris and Purdy 1987,
Lew et al. 1991). Severe drooling often necessitates frequent
clothing changes and can cause damage to books, electronic
equipment, and other educational materials. The most unfortunate consequence of drooling may be social isolation. Because
drooling is unsightly and produces an unpleasant odor, people may avoid individuals who drool and physical contact
may be reduced. Isolation, in turn, can have devastating effects
on self-esteem (Blasco and Allaire 1992).
Why some individuals with CP drool and others do not is not
entirely clear. Some researchers have suggested that drooling
is caused by disordered oral motor functioning (Burgmayer
and Jung 1983, O’Dwyer et al. 1989, Lespargot et al. 1993).
Others have noted that ‘Drooling might be due to hypersalivation and/or an insufficient mechanism for the removal of
saliva’ (Ekedahl et al. 1974, p 78).
Researchers thus far have determined that children who
drool have increased difficulty forming a bolus (Ekedahl et al.
1974), reduced lip closure, slightly less intraoral suction, and
more oral residue after the swallow (Lespargot et al. 1993).
Significant negative correlations have been found between
drooling and sucking ability, drooling and chewing ability,
and drooling and swallowing. Significant positive correlations
have been found between drooling and poor head control,
reduced ability to voluntarily control the lips, reduced ability to voluntarily control the tongue, and reduced ability to
voluntarily control the jaw (Van De Heyning et al. 1980).
Other investigators have found reduced intraoral sensitivity
(Weiss-Lambrou et al. 1988), reduced frequency of spontaneous swallowing (Sochaniwskyj et al. 1986), esophageal
stage abnormalities (Ekedahl et al. 1974), dental malocclusion
(Franklin et al. 1996), and poor coordination of obicularis
oris and masseter activity (Sochaniwskyj et al. 1986) in children who drool.
Little is known about saliva production in children with
CP. Davis (1979) found significantly lower stimulated parotid
flow rates in children with moderate to severe head and neck
involvement due to spastic CP than in control participants.
Chiat and Kessler (1979) noted that the excised submandibular glands of drooling patients with CP appeared hypertrophied and enlarged.
Recently, Tahmassebi and Curzon (2003) attempted to
address the role saliva production plays in drooling in children
with CP. They concluded that children with CP who drooled
did not produce excess saliva; however, their data must be
interpreted with caution because of significant methodological limitations. The authors compared children with CP with
typically developing age- and sex-matched peers, but ‘the
method and duration of saliva collection in the control group
was chosen to be different from those of CP individuals’ (p 109).
In the control participants, the researchers used a ‘draining
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2004, 46: 801–806 801
method’, an unstimulated method of whole saliva collection.
With the study participants, on the other hand, they used a
chin cup described by Sochaniwskyj in 1982. Each participant
participated in only one 15-minute collection system. It should
be noted that the system described in Sochaniwskyj’s study is
described as a method of quantifying drooling, not whole saliva
production. The chin cup only measures saliva spilled and does
not account for saliva remaining in the oral cavity. Furthermore,
Sochaniwskyj noted, ‘Based on experience from this study at
least five 30-minute sessions must be conducted with a participant in order to obtain a true estimate of drooling rates and
variability’ (Sochaniwskyj 1982, p 607).
Unfortunately, despite the fact that limited data exist about
the amount of saliva produced by individuals with CP who
drool, many drooling treatments, including surgical management, medications, and injections of botulinum toxin A, are
designed to reduce the amount of saliva produced. The purpose of this study was to help us define those factors that influence drooling.
Method
Participants and controls were recruited from several different
schools, therapy centers, and residential facilities serving individuals with CP in the Chicago area. Letters were sent to professionals who in turn referred appropriate candidates after
securing informed consent. Three groups were examined in
this investigation: (1) 14 participants with CP who drooled
(CP+); (2) 14 participants with CP who did not drool (CP–);
and (3) 14 typically developing participants (CTRL). The study
protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board
of Northwestern University. Participants (six females, eight
males) ranging in age from 7 years 11 months to 18 years 2
months (mean 11y 8mo, SD 3y 5mo) with a confirmed medical diagnosis of spastic CP served as study participants (see
Table I). All participants with CP+ met the following criteria:
(1) exhibited severe to profuse and frequent to constant
drooling (as determined by questionnaire); (2) no medication
to reduce saliva production for at least 72h; (3) no history of
head or neck radiation; (4) no ulcerations in the oral cavity;
and (5) no surgical procedure to reduce drooling. Each parent
was required to complete a questionnaire that contained
questions about drooling severity and frequency (ThomasStonell and Greenberg 1988), presence of factors related to
drooling like open mouth posture and poor head control (Van
De Heyning et al. 1980), presence of factors related to saliva
production (e.g. medications, gastroesophageal reflux), cognitive ability, feeding and swallowing ability, and severity of
motor involvement (Gisel and Alphonce 1995).
On the questionnaire, each drooling severity level and frequency related to a number on an ordinal scale from one to
five (e.g. moderate has a score of three, severe has a score of
Table I: Distribution of cerebral palsy types
Diplegia
Hemiplegia
Quadriplegia
CP+
5
1
8
CP–
5
3
6
CP+, participants with CP and drooling; CP–, participants with CP
without drooling.
802 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2004, 46: 801–806
four, and profuse has a score of five). Severity and frequency
numbers were added for each participant to yield a drooling
quotient. Participants with a drooling quotient score of 6 to 10
were included as participants. Fourteen age- and sex-matched
controls were included in each of two groups: (1) participants with CP+ who were dry to mild and never to infrequent droolers, as determined by questionnaire (CP–); and
(2) typically developing peers (CTRL). The cumulative drooling quotient scores for control participants were in the range
two to four.
The mean difference in age between CP+ and CP– groups
was 7.85 months, and the mean difference in age between
CP+ and CTRL groups was 6.79 months. Control participants
also met criteria 2 through 5 above.
Before the evaluation session, each participant’s relevant
medical records were obtained to confirm the diagnosis of CP+
and provide any information on past evaluation or treatment
of drooling or swallowing. In addition to completing the abovementioned questionnaire, the Pediatric Evaluation of Disability
Inventory (PEDI) was also administered. The PEDI is a clinical assessment instrument that assesses children’s functional
abilities in a variety of areas including self-care, mobility, and
social function (Feldman et al. 1990; Haley et al. 1991, 1992).
Information from the questionnaire and PEDI, with a videotape of the evaluation session, were used to assess severity of
CP according to the Gross Motor Function Classification System
(GMFCS) for CP (Palisano et al. 1997).
A second researcher independently assigned GMFCS scores
to nine of the participants to establish interrater reliability.
Reliability analysis of these data revealed good interobserver
agreement (Spearman’s rho correlation=0.94, p<0.0001).
PEDI scaled scores were calculated for the functional skills
sections of the self-care, mobility, and social function domains.
The mean of the three domains was calculated to yield the
‘PEDI functional skills average’ score, which was used to compare performance among the three groups.
Questionnaire information and observation of participant
were also used to determine severity of dysarthria as measured on a five-point scale (Yorkston et al. 1999). Scores were
assigned as follows: (1) no detectable speech disorder; (2)
obvious speech disorder with intelligible speech; (3) reduction in intelligibility; (4) natural speech supplemented with
augmentative and alternative communication techniques;
(5) no functional speech.
The evaluation session itself consisted of three sections.
On the day of the evaluation session, parents of participants
were instructed not to allow their child to eat or drink anything or chew gum for 2 hours before the procedure. In addition, if a participant developed a cold, flu, or upper respiratory
infection, parents were instructed to reschedule.
To quantify the amount of saliva the participants produced,
the Saxon test (Kohler and Winter 1985), a simple gauzechewing procedure, was performed by each participant.
Participants with large oral cavities used a 10.16cm × 10.16cm
sterile piece of gauze (4inch × 4inch Curity, 12-ply, Kendall
Co, Boston, MA) that was folded twice at 90˚ angles and
placed in a plastic container. A 7.62cm × 7.62cm (3inch ×
3inch) piece of gauze was substituted in younger participants
or in participants with smaller oral cavities. The gauze and
plastic container were weighed using a Mettler P-1200 balance (Mettler, Columbus, OH). Before each evaluation session, the balance was calibrated with a 1mg weight. Participants
were asked to swallow to remove any preexisting oral fluid. All
participants and control participants then had their oral cavities suctioned with a portable oral suction unit fitted with sterile suction tubing and a straight Yangkauer sterile suction
handle. Saliva was collected by having each participant chew
on the gauze for exactly one minute ‘as if chewing on food’.
To avoid the risk of potential aspiration of the gauze, a string
of dental floss was threaded through the gauze and held by
the examiner while the participant was chewing. After the
chewing procedure, the gauze was replaced in the same plastic container and weighed. The amount of saliva produced
was determined by subtracting the original weight from the
weight obtained after chewing. During the Saxon test, each
participant wore a pre-weighed disposable Sani-Tab ChainFree Polyback dental towel (Crosstex, New York). If any saliva
was lost from the mouth, this bib was used to catch and/or
wipe it. After the gauze-chewing procedure was completed,
the bib was weighed and the quantity of saliva in the bib was
added to the quantity in the gauze to yield the total amount
of saliva produced.
To determine whether multiple trials of the Saxon test
might be needed, the variability in data from the Kohler and
Winter (1985) study were subjected to Spearman ranked correlations. Ten of their participants were tested on three occasions, 7 to 14 days apart. The mean correlation was calculated
(Spearman’s rho correlation=0.90). Mulligan et al. (1995) also
found test–retest reliability of this measurement to be high
(r=0.80, p<0.001) and results have been found to be similar
to other stimulated whole-saliva collection techniques.
Frequency of spontaneous swallowing during a 20-minute
period was determined using concurrent cervical auscultation
and videotaped observation of laryngeal elevation. Cervical
auscultation is a non-invasive technique for listening to the
sounds of pharyngeal swallow. This technique has been used
to assess adults and children for clinical and research purposes (Vice et al. 1990, Takahashi et al. 1994, Zenner et al.
1995). Each participant’s neck was cleaned with alcohol, and a
tie-clip microphone was affixed to the midline of the neck
around the level of the cricoid cartilage using a double-sided
electrode washer (Cichero and Murdoch 2002). The microphone was connected to the audio input channel of the video
camera. The camcorder was positioned at a level slightly lower
than each participant’s neck and angled upward to allow maximum visualization of laryngeal elevation. During this 20minute period, the participants were each shown the same
children’s video to help distract them from concerns with the
equipment as well as to provide an opportunity to observe
swallowing during a familiar childhood activity (i.e. watching
television). The videotapes of the evaluation session were
reviewed by the principal investigator and 25% were reviewed
again to establish intraobserver reliability. Reliability analysis of
these data revealed good intraobserver agreement (r=0.9959,
p<0.01). A second observer reviewed 45% of the videotapes.
Interobserver agreement was also high (r=0.9962, p<0.01). A
swallow was defined as visual observation of laryngeal elevation accompanied by the ‘clunk’ sound associated with opening of the upper esophageal sphincter (Hamlet et al. 1990).
The number of swallows per 20-minute period was noted.
While frequency of swallowing was being monitored, the
amount of drooling was measured by using the bib-weighing
technique. A disposable Sani-Tab Chain-Free Polyback dental
towel was placed in a plastic container and weighed. Before
the bib-weighing procedure, each participant’s mouth and
chin were wiped with a paper towel to remove any drool
present before the procedure. Next, each participant wore
the bib for exactly 10 minutes. After the 10-minute period,
any saliva on the lips or chin was wiped with the bib. The bib
was replaced in the plastic container and weighed. The
amount of drool produced was determined by subtracting
the original bib weight from the weight obtained after the 10minute period.
Finally, each participant was given the Test of Nonverbal
Intelligence-3 (TONI-3). TONI-3 consists of 50 questions, and
administration takes about 15 minutes. Participants were
required to look at a series of pictures and select appropriate
responses with pointing or eye-gaze. No verbal responses were
required. Standard scores were computed for the TONI-3.
Results
Drooling severity as measured by questionnaire (drooling
quotient) and the bib weighing technique were compared
using Spearman’s rho correlation. Results indicated that there
was a positive correlation between caregiver reports of drooling severity and bib weight during the examination session
(Spearman’s rho correlation=0.604, p<0.05).
All of the participants tolerated the Saxon test well, without any overt signs of anxiety such as protest or crying; however, two participants in the CP+ group and three participants
in the CP– group gagged on the gauze owing to intraoral
hypersensitivity. None of the typically developing participants
gagged during the Saxon test. The impact of gagging on saliva
production is not clear. On this test, differences were not
determined to be statistically significant by an analysis of variance (ANOVA F(2,39)=2.842, p=0.071; see Fig. 1). No correlation
was found between amount of saliva produced and amount
drooled (r=0.245). Lip incompetence during chewing, as
evidenced by gauze protruding out of the mouth, was
observed in all of the CP+ participants and 11 of the 14 participants in the CP– group during the Saxon test. All of the
typically developing control participants were able to achieve
complete lip closure around the gauze. The control participants also demonstrated the ability to form a bolus with the
gauze and, at the end of the 60 seconds, the participants spat
out rounded gauze balls. All of the participants in the CP+
group and 11 of the participants in the CP– group were unable
to form a bolus (e.g. the gauze was still square in shape), suggesting poor oral coordination.
Both groups of participants with CP tended to swallow less
frequently than typically developing participants, with participants in the CP– group swallowing more frequently than participants in the CP+ group (Fig. 2). However, an ANOVA revealed
no statistically significant difference between the three groups
(F(2,39)=1.8274, p=0.1743).
An ANOVA conducted on the PEDI functional skills average scores indicated significant differences between the three
groups (F(2,39)=23.522, p<0.0001). A Tukey/Kramer procedure revealed statistically significant differences in functional
skills, with typically developing participants demonstrating
higher functional skill scores than both groups of participants with CP (p<0.01), and participants with CP who did
not drool (CP–) demonstrating higher functional skill scores
than participants with CP who drooled, i.e. CP+ (p<0.05).
Likewise, an ANOVA conducted on the TONI-3 scores
revealed statistically significant differences between the
Drooling, Saliva Production, and Swallowing in Cerebral Palsy Jill E Senner et al. 803
three groups (F (2,39) =31.761, p<0.0001). A Tukey–Kramer
procedure revealed statistically significant differences in
nonverbal intelligence, with typically developing participants demonstrating higher nonverbal intelligence than both
groups of participants with CP (p<0.01), and participants in
the CP– group demonstrating higher nonverbal intelligence
scores than participants in the CP+ group (p<0.01). Three
participants in the CP+ group were unable to be tested using
the TONI-3 because of severe motor involvement and reduced
cognitive abilities. These participants were assigned scores of
60, which is the lowest possible standard score.
In the CP+ group, one participant had a GMFCS score of 1,
four participants received a score of 2, two received a score of
3, three received a score of 4, and four participants received a
score of 5. In the CP– group, four participants received a GMFCS
score of 1, four received a score of 2, two received a score of 3,
three received a score of 4, and one participant received a score
of 5. A Spearman’s rho correlation indicated that gross motor
severity was not correlated with drooling severity as measured
by the questionnaire (drooling quotient).
Eight of 14 of the participants in the CP+ group used
some formal augmentative and alternative communication
system (e.g. picture boards, sign language, voice output communication aids) whereas none of the participants in the CP–
group was reported to use any forms of augmentative and
alternative communication. A Fisher’s exact test indicated significant differences between the groups (p-exact<0.001).
In the CP+ group, no participant had a dysarthria score of 1,
three participants received a score of 2, three participants
received a score of 3, three participants received a score of
4, and five participants received a score of 5. In the CP– group,
five participants received a dysarthria score of 1, nine received
a score of 2, none received a score of 3, 4, or 5. Drooling quotient scores were found to be positively correlated with
dysarthria severity scores (Spearman’s rho correlation=0.82,
p<0.0001).
On the questionnaire, caregivers were also asked whether
or not participants had experienced seizures. Eight out of 14
participants in the CP+ group had been diagnosed with
seizure activity. Only 1 of 14 participants in the CP– group
had been diagnosed with seizure activity. A Fisher exact test
indicated significant differences between the groups (pexact=0.006086) for this item. All of the participants who
experienced seizures were managed with anticonvulsant
medications including carbamazepine, phenobarbital, divalporex sodium, and clonazepam.
Discussion
Results of the present study suggest children with CP who
drool have poorer functional skills scores, lower nonverbal
intelligence scores, more severe oral motor involvement,
and a tendency to swallow less frequently than children with
CP who don’t drool. Participants with CP did not produce
excess saliva, and no correlation was found between amount
of saliva produced and amount drooled, suggesting that hypersalivation is not one of the factors responsible for drooling in
this group.
As previously noted, data from this study indicate that participants with CP do not produce excess saliva. However, in
this study, there was a motor component (i.e. chewing)
involved in the Saxon saliva collection, that may have put
participants with CP at a disadvantage. Despite methodological limitations, these results are consistent with those of Davis
(1979), who found significantly lower stimulated parotid
flow rates in children with moderate to severe head and neck
involvement due to CP than in control participants. The
results are also consistent with those of Tahmassebi and
Curzon (2003), who found that children who drooled did
not produce excess saliva. Furthermore, adult patients with
Parkinson’s disease have also been demonstrated to produce
significantly less unstimulated saliva than age- and sexmatched control participants (Bagheri et al. 1999), suggesting
Frequency of swallowing
Saxon test
50
CP with drooling
CP without drooling
2.5
Control
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Figure 1: Comparison of saliva production among groups.
804 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2004, 46: 801–806
Mean number of swallows in 20 minutes
Mean stimulated whole saliva production
per minute (grams)
3
CP with drooling
CP without drooling
Control
40
30
20
10
0
Figure 2: Comparison of swallowing frequency among groups.
that drooling in these patients is related to swallowing difficulties rather than hypersalivation. It is important to note that
both the present study and Davis (1979) measured stimulated saliva production. Future studies using unstimulated collection techniques would be helpful in determining factors
contributing to reduced saliva production.
Furthermore, in the present study no correlation was found
between stimulated whole saliva production and amount
drooled. These findings are of particular importance because
many drooling treatments, including surgical management,
medications, and injections of botulinum toxin A, are designed
to reduce the amount of saliva produced. Although most
studies have not termed the post-treatment reduction in
salivary flow ‘xerostomia’ as such, reduction in salivary flow
has not been without consequence. In the literature, several
studies have reported significant increases in dental caries
after surgical intervention (Arnrup and Crossner 1990,
Hallett et al. 1995).
Furthermore, it has been estimated that about a third of
children with clinically significant drooling demonstrate evidence of gastroesophageal reflux (Heine et al. 1996). In this
study, three of 14 of participants in the CP+ group were found
to have gastroesophageal reflux, as determined by review of
medical records and analysis of the intake questionnaire.
Saliva plays an important role in the defense against the acidinduced esophageal mucosal injury that can occur with gastroesophageal reflux. In children who drool, constant saliva
loss may already impair clearance of refluxed gastric acid from
the esophagus, which in turn may perpetuate esophageal dysmotility and esophagitis (Heine et al. 1996). Further reduction
in saliva production caused by drooling treatments might
only serve to exacerbate this situation.
This is not to say that reducing saliva is inappropriate.
Many researchers have reported positive results in their
patients following surgical procedures as determined by
subjective rating scales. (O’Dwyer et al. 1989, Ethunandan
and Macpherson 1998, Mankarious et al. 1999). For example, surgical procedures have been effective in eliminating
episodes of aspiration pneumonia secondary to aspirated
salivary secretions (Klem and Mair 1999). Nonetheless, it
would seem that using more stringent candidacy criteria for
saliva reduction treatments would be prudent. As a minimum, before treatment, a comprehensive examination of
oropharyngeal swallowing as well as gastroesophageal reflux
should be completed. The need for saliva reduction procedures would best be determined by an interdisciplinary team.
The team could also monitor and possibly prevent potential
sequelae with appropriate medical management. It has been
suggested that team management using an otolaryngologist, a
speech-language pathologist, and a pediatric dentist works
well for children with developmental disabilities who drool
(Crysdale 1992). Because of the medically complex nature
of these children, a pediatric physician could also play an
important role.
Although the data did not achieve statistical significance,
there was also a tendency for participants with CP to swallow
less frequently than typically developing participants, with
participants in the CP– group swallowing more frequently
than participants in the CP+ group (Fig. 2). These results are
consistent with a previous study conducted by Sochaniwskyj
et al. (1986, p 871) which found ‘The nondrooling group
swallowed at approximately 75% the rate of normals, while
the drooling group swallowed at 45% the normal rate’.
Although Sochaniwskyj et al. (1986) concluded that drooling
in children with CP was caused by ‘both inefficient and infrequent swallowing’, correlation does not imply causation.
Previous studies have established that both saliva flow rate
and volume are important variables in determining spontaneous swallowing frequency (Kapila et al. 1984, Rudney et
al. 1995, Pouderoux et al. 1996). It is still unclear whether
reduced swallowing rates cause drooling or whether reduced
swallowing rates are the result of reduced saliva production
coupled with the anterior loss of saliva that occurs in drooling. Researchers have suggested that ‘posterior drooling’, or
pooling of saliva in the pharynx, might occur in patients with
CP who drool (Blasco and Allaire 1992); however, so far,
there has only been indirect evidence of this (e.g. wet, gurgling vocal quality). Further studies of children with CP who
drool are needed, to investigate the amount of saliva reaching the pharynx and what happens to that saliva once it
reaches the pharynx.
It is clear that drooling is caused by multiple factors including poor oral motor abilities, poor intraoral sensations, and
dysphagia. Results of the present study indicate that hypersalivation is not one of the factors responsible for drooling in
children with CP. Because of the complex nature of drooling
in children with CP, each child’s individual strengths and areas
of need should be assessed by a multidisciplinary team and
treatment should be determined on a case-by-case basis.
Although this study suggests that children who drool have
more severe forms of CP and a higher probability of having a
lower IQ, this does not mean that they cannot benefit from
treatment. Treatments aimed at increasing swallowing frequency and efficiency as well as treatments to improve
impaired head and neck control may be fruitful endeavors.
Characteristics of the population of children with CP who
drool, particularly poorer oral motor control and generally
lower cognitive skills, make treatments involving following
complex directives or completing oral exercises difficult.
However, not all treatments require active patient participation. Recent reports in the literature about treatments such as
the use of intraoral appliances (Inga et al. 2001) and tongue
acupuncture (Wong et al. 2001) may hold promise and should
be investigated further.
DOI: 10.1017/S0012162204001409
Accepted for publication 23rd March 2004.
Acknowledgements
This work was partly supported by the Dissertation Year Grant from
the University Research Grants Committee at Northwestern University.
References
Arnrup K, Crossner C-G. (1990) Caries prevalence after
submandibular duct retroposition in drooling children with
neurological disorders. Pediatr Dent 12: 98–101.
Bagheri H, Damase-Michel C, Lapeyre-Mestre M, Cismondo S,
O’Connell D, Senard J-M, Rascol O, Montastruc J-L. (1999)
A study of salivary secretion in Parkinson’s Disease. Clin
Neuropharmacol 22: 213–215.
Blasco P, Allaire J. (1992) Drooling in the developmentally disabled:
management practices and recommendations. Dev Med Child
Neurol 34: 849–862.
Brodsky L. (1993) Drooling in Children. In: Arvedson JC, Brodsky L,
editors. Pediatric Swallowing and Feeding: Assessment and
Management. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group. p 389–416.
Drooling, Saliva Production, and Swallowing in Cerebral Palsy Jill E Senner et al. 805
Burgmayer S, Jung H. (1983) Hypersalivation in severe mental
retardation. Int J Rehabil Res 6: 193–197.
Chiat LA, Kessler E. (1979) An anti-drooling operation in cerebral
palsy. S Afr Med J 56: 676–678.
Cichero JAY, Murdoch BE. (2002) Detection of swallowing sounds:
methodology revisited. Dysphagia 17: 40–49.
Cotton RT, Richardson MA. (1981) The effect of submandibular duct
rerouting in the treatment of sialorrhea in children. Otolaryngol
Head Neck Surg 89: 535–541.
Crysdale WS. (1992) Experience with team assessment and
management. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 31: 77–80.
Davis MJ. (1979) Parotid salivary secretion and composition in
cerebral palsy. J Dent Res 58: 1808. (Abstract)
Ekedahl C, Mansson I, Sandberg N. (1974) Swallowing dysfunction in
the brain damaged with drooling. Acta Otolaryngol 78: 141–149.
Ethunandan M, Macpherson D. (1998) Persistent drooling:
treatment by bilateral submandibular duct transposition and
simultaneous sublingual gland excision. Ann R Coll Surg Engl
80: 279–282.
Feldman AB, Haley SM, Coryell J. (1990) Concurrent and construct
validity of the Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory. Phys
Ther 70: 602–610.
Franklin DL, Luther F, Curzon MEJ. (1996) The prevalence of
malocclusion in children with cerebral palsy. Eur J Orthod
18: 637–643.
Gisel EG, Alphonce E. (1995) Classification of eating impairments
based on eating efficiency in children with cerebral palsy.
Dysphagia 10: 268–274.
Haley SM, Coster WJ, Ludlow LH. (1991) Pediatric functional
outcome measures. Pediatr Rehabil 2: 689–723.
Haley SM, Faas RM, Coster WJ. (1992) Pediatric Evaluation of
Disability Inventory. Development, Standardization and
Administration Manual. Boston, MA: New England Medical
Center Hospital.
Hallett KB, Lucas JO, Johnston T, Reddihough D, Hall RK. (1995)
Dental health of children with cerebral palsy following
sialodochoplasty. Spec Care Dent 15: 234–238.
Hamlet SL, Nelson RJ, Patterson RL. (1990) Interpreting the sounds
of swallowing: fluid flow through the cricopharyngeus. Ann Otol
Rhinol Laryngol 99: 749–752.
Harris S, Purdy A. (1987) Drooling and its management in cerebral
palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 29: 805–814.
Heine RG, Catto-Smith AG, Reddihough DS. (1996) Effect of
antireflux medication on salivary drooling in children with
cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 38: 1030–1036.
Inga CJ, Reddy AK, Richardson SA, Sanders B. (2001) Appliance
for chronic drooling in cerebral palsy patients. Pediatr Dent
23: 241–242.
Johnson H, Scott A. (1993) A Practical Approach to Saliva Control.
Tuscon, AZ: Communication Skill Builders.
Kapila YV, Dodds WJ, Helm JF, Hogan WJ. (1984) Relationship
between swallow rate and salivary flow. Dig Dis Sci 29: 528–533.
Klem C, Mair EA. (1999) Four-duct ligation: a simple and effective
treatment for chronic aspiration from sialorrhea. Arch
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 125: 796–800.
Kohler PF, Winter ME. (1985) A quantitative test for xerostomia.
Arthritis Rheum 28: 1128–1132.
Lespargot A, Langevin M, Muller S, Guillemont S. (1993)
Swallowing disturbances associated with drooling in cerebralpalsied children. Dev Med Child Neurol 35: 298–304.
Lew KM, Younis RT, Lazar RH. (1991) The current management of
sialorrhea. Ear Nose Throat J 70: 99–105.
Mankarious L, Bottrill I, Huchzermyer P, Bailey M. (1999) Long-term
follow-up of submandibular duct rerouting for the treatment of
sialorrhea in the pediatric population. Otolaryngol Head Neck
Surg 120: 303–307.
806 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2004, 46: 801–806
Morris SE, Klein MD. (1987) Pre-Feeding Skills: A Comprehensive
Resource for Feeding Development. Tucson, AZ: Communication
Skill Builders.
Mulligan R, Navazesh M, Wood GJ. (1995) A pilot study comparing
three salivary collection methods in an adult population with
salivary gland hypofunction. Spec Care Dent 15: 154–157.
O’Dwyer TP, Timon C, Walsh MA. (1989) Surgical management of
drooling in the neurologically damaged child. J Laryngol Otol
103: 750–752.
Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell P, Wood E, Galuppi B.
(1997) Gross Motor Function Classification System for Cerebral
Palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 39: 214–223.
Pouderoux P, Logemann JA, Kahrilas P. (1996) Pharyngeal swallow
elicited by fluid infusion: role of volition and vallecular
containment. Am J Physiol 270: G347–G354.
Rudney JD, Ji Z, Larson CJ. (1995) The prediction of saliva
swallowing frequency in humans from estimates of salivary flow
rate and from the volume of saliva swallowed. Arch Oral Biol
40: 507–512.
Sochaniwskyj A. (1982) Drool quantification: noninvasive
technique. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 63: 605–606.
Sochaniwskyj A, Koheil R, Bablich K, Milner M, Kenny D. (1986)
Oral motor functioning, frequency of swallowing and drooling in
normal children and in children with cerebral palsy. Arch Phys
Med Rehabil 67: 866–874.
Takahashi K, Groher ME, Michi K. (1994) Methodology for detecting
swallowing sounds. Dysphagia 9: 54–62.
Tahmassebi JF, Curzon MEJ. (2003) The cause of drooling in
children with cerebral palsy – hypersalivation or swallowing
defect? Int J Pediatr Dent 13: 106–111.
Thomas-Stonell N, Greenberg J. (1988) Three treatment
approaches and clinical factors in the reduction of drooling.
Dysphagia 3: 73–78.
Van De Heyning P, Marquet J, Creten W. (1980) Drooling in
children with cerebral palsy. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Belg
34: 691–705.
Vice FL, Heinz HM, Giuriati G, Hood M, Bosma JF. (1990) Cervical
auscultation of suckle feeding in newborn infants. Dev Med Child
Neurol 32: 760–768.
Weiss-Lambrou R, Tetreault S, Dudley J. (1988) The relationship
between oral sensation and drooling in persons with cerebral
palsy. Am J Occup Ther 43: 155–161.
Wong V, Sun JG, Wong W. (2001) Traditional Chinese medicine
(tongue acupuncture) in children with drooling problems.
Pediatr Neurol 25: 47–54.
Yorkston K, Beukelman D, Strand E, Bell K. (1999) Management of
Motor Speech Disorders in Children and Adults. 2nd edn. Austin,
TX: Pro Ed.
Zenner PM, Losinski DS, Mills RH. (1995) Using cervical
auscultation in the clinical dysphagia examination in long-term
care. Dysphagia 10: 27–31.
List of abbreviations
CP+
CP–
CTRL
GMFCS
PEDI
TONI-3
Participants with cerebral palsy and drooling
Participants with cerebral palsy without drooling
Control participants
Gross Motor Function Classification System
Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-3